Many of us fear that text messaging
and the use of “textese” may be adversely affecting children and adults’ use of
Standard English. The media has promoted
these fears with anecdotal reports given by educators of children using
textisms in their formal writing, and, in some cases, by supervisors showing
concern for their employees’ use of these abbreviations in their professional
writing (Lee, 2002; Barker, 2007; Rogers, 2008). According to recent research
on the topic, these individuals may be the outliers, and texting and textisms
may actually serve as a way to increase reading skills, literacy, and spelling
fluency.
Or does it? |
What
is textese? What is a textism?
Textese
is an abbreviated vocabulary that includes initialisms, letter/number
homophones, contractions or shortenings of words or phrases, emoticons (symbols
for representing emotions such as L
for sad), and the deletions of unnecessary words, vowels, punctuation, and
capitalization (Thurlow, 2003; Carrington, 2004; Varnhagen et al., 2010). This
vocabulary stems from text messages originally only being able to contain up to
120 characters, and forcing users to form a more economical mode of
communication within this limitation. Abbreviations such as “2nite” for tonight
and “gr8” for great are common occurrences in this abbreviated language. The
language does not end with just words, but some of the most common “textisms”
are often whole phrases, such as “lol” for laugh out loud, “C u l8r” for see
you later, or even “omw” for on my way. These abbreviated phrase can even be as
complicated as “idc wots ur add cwot” meaning I don’t care what your address is
– complete waste of time. Maybe phrases
such as this one are why scholars deemed to give these communications the name
textese, because it reads almost like a language of its own.
To
some these may seem quite foreign and a complete bastardization of the language
we speak on a daily basis, but increasingly it is becoming more and more common
for people to communicate using these textisms. Text messaging surpassed voice
calls in popularity as a means to communicate while mobile in 2008, and has
been on the rise ever since (Reardon, 2008; Drouin, 2011).
Research
Theories
The
research having been done on texting and literacy is not vast, nor clear cut.
There are several theories that attempt to explain the impact that texting and
the use of textese is having on adults and children’s reading, spelling, and
language fluency. Some theories related to psychological theories on memory.
Drouin and Davis
(2009) suggested that the theories of retroactive interference and decay may be
at play. This was hypotesized when the research was assumed to follow the
anecdotal evidence provided by the media. Retroactive interference suggests that
information presented at a later time may interfere with information presented
at an earlier time (McGeoch, 1932; Britt, 1935). Meanwhile, decay theory states
that learned information that is not accessed may be less accessible over time
(Brown, 1958). Dourin and Davis (2009) applied this to the scenario of textisms
and hypothesized that exposure to the textism might make is more difficult to
remember the Standard English spelling of words. Alternatively, in cases where
the Standard English version of a word is not accessed over a long period of
time, it would become more difficult to remember. There theory gained initial support for
previous research examining processes with regard to spelling in adults has
shown that even a single exposure to a misspelled word can have a detrimental
impact on future spellings of that word (Jacoby & Hollingshead, 1990; Dixon
& Kamiska, 1997; Kratz & Frost, 2001). Further, this effect is
especially pronounced when a misspelled word is a plausible phonological
alternative (or it looks the way it sounds) (Kratz & Frost, 2001). Thus, it
could indeed be possible that continued exposure to textism (interference)
could lead to forgetting (or decay) of the Standard English presentation.
However, recent
research has not been able to lend support to these theories of memory
interference, and, in fact, it has shown just the opposite to be the case, text
messaging may actually be positively associated with children and adult
literacy (Plester et al., 2008, 2009). There have been two broad theories to
explain these findings. First, Plester et al. (2009) suggested that texting can
allow people access to a form of written language which is not constrained by
standard grammer and spelling or produced for the purposes of formal learning practice
in school, but rather as a means of easy communication with frieds. Crystal
(2008) and Leake (2008) further hypothesized that this freeing from
conventional constraints and the additional necessity of brevity to fall within
the character limit allows children and adults to use reading and writing in a
much more playful way.
These
theories postulate that there may be something about the very nature of texting
and textisms that help with literacy (Powell & Dixon, 2011). In order to
read or produce textisms, one has to have a good level of phonological
awareness, that is, sensitivity to the underlying sound structure of spoken
language. The fact that texting could be mediated by phonological processes
suggests that it may be linked to phonological awareness, which had been
repeatedly shown to predict literacy measures (Powell & Dixon, 2011; Adams,
1990).
Two
Research Studies
Dixon
and Powell (2011) conducted a study using 94 undergraduate students where
participants were exposed to misspellings, correct spellings, and textisms.
Participants were given a pre- and post- test measure on spelling ability.
During this one exposure, significant positive effects on posttest scores could
be seen in both the correct spelling exposure group and the textism exposure
group. This study provides experimental evidence that even a single exposure to
textisms can have a positive effect on knowledge of words and standard
spellings.
Drouin
(2011) conducted another study that had some other interesting findings. In this
study, 152 undergraduate students engaged in grammar exercises, literacy tasks
measuring reading, reading fluency, and spelling, followed by a survey on the
use of text messaging, textese use in different contents, and access to social
networking sites. The results show that participants in this study reported
using textese more often as compared to previous research by Drouin and Davis
(2009). However, although the overall use of textese may have increased in the
past couple of years, they continued to mediate use of textese by context. For
example, participants reported using textese more often in text messages and
emails to friends, but rarely on social networking sites and in emails to
professors (Drouin, 2011). These results suggest that people are making
conscious decisions on whether or not to use textese.
This
study supports an interesting notion: that people are constantly able to make
the conscious decision to switch between textese and Standard English without
one interfering directly with the other. Some have suggested that this is the
equivalent of a bilingual person switching between two languages, while there
may be some crossover an individual is easily able to correct any mistakes
(Drouin, 2011). It may also help to note
that most of the crossover experienced by bilingual person is in speech, and as
textese is almost a purely written language these effects should be limited.
So
why are we still seeing textese in formal writing and in emails to professors?
In Drouin (2011), there was a significant correlation among participants that
used textese more on social networking sites and in emails to professors and
lower reading accuracy scores. This suggests that those without a comprehensive
understanding of Standard English are the ones most likely to commit these
context faux pas. Additionally, this mediated use by context supports another
interesting notion. Participants reported that their reason for not using
textese on social networking sites or in emails to professors was that the context
was not appropriate. Therefore, those who do use textese in these contexts may
not view them as inappropriate, indicating a more limited pragmatic knowledge
(Drouin, 2011).
These
research finding show that it is not texting or textese itself that is causing
a decline in language standards, but maybe a more fundamental educational flaw.
These results support that if a child or adult has a strong basis in Standard
English it is hard to reverse that knowledge with the use of textese.
Additionally, it may actually be beneficial for remembering the correct
spelling of a word, and continuing in lifelong reading fluency skills. There is
also research that supports texting and textism as a basis for teaching the
founding principles of Standard English, but more on that later.
Sources
Adams M.J. (1990) Beginning to Read: Thinking and Learn- ing about
Print. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
Barker,
I. (2007). Txts r gr8 but not in exams. Times
Educational Supplement 4723, 20. 9 Feburary. Retrieved from: http://www.tes.co.uk/article.aspx?storycode=2341958
Britt S.H. (1935) Retroactive inhibition: a review of the litera- ture. Psychological
Bulletin 32, 381–440.
Brown J. (1958) Some tests of the decay theory of immediate memory. Quarterly
Journal of Experimental Psychology 10, 12–21.
Carrington,
V. (2004). Texts and literacies of the Shi Jinrui. British Journal of Sociology Education 25, 215-228. Doi: 10.1080/0142569042000205109.
Crystal D. (2008) Txting: The Gr8 Db8. Oxford University Press,
Oxford, UK.
Dixon M. & Kaminska Z. (1997) The spell of misspelled words:
susceptibility to orthographic priming as a function of spelling proficiency. Reading
and Writing: An Interdisci- plinary Journal 9, 483–498.
Drouin M. & Davis C. (2009) R U txting? Is the use of text speak
hurting your literacy? Journal of Literacy Research 41, 46–67.
Drouin,
M.A. (2011). College students’ text messaging, use of textese, and literacy
skills. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning
27, 67-75. Doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2729.2010.00399.x
Jacoby L.L. & Hollingshead A. (1990) Reading student essays may be
hazardous to your spelling: effects of reading incor- rectly and correctly
spelled words. Canadian Journal of Psychology 44, 345–358.
Katz L. & Frost S.J. (2001) Phonology constrains the internal
orthographic lexicon. Reading and Writing: An Interdisci- plinary Journal 14,
297–332.
Leake J. (2008 May 25) Texting boosts children’s literacy. The Sunday
Times. Available at: http://www.timesonline.co.uk/tol/news/uk/education/article3998970.ece
Lee,
J. (2002). Nu shortcuts in school r 2
much 4 teachers. The New York Times 19
September. Retrieved from: http://www.nytimes.com/2002/09/19/technology/circuits/19MESS.html#
McGeoch J.A. (1932) Forgetting and the law of disuse. Psychological
Review 39, 352–370.
Plester B., Wood C. & Bell L. (2008) Txt msg n school literacy: does
texting and knowledge of text abbrev- iations adversely affect children’s
literacy attainment? Lit- eracy 42, 137–144. doi:
10.1111/j.1741-4369.2008. 00489.x.
Plester B., Wood C. & Joshi P. (2009) Exploring the relation- ship
between children’s knowledge of text message abbre- viations and school
literacy outcomes. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 27,
145–161. doi: 10.1348/ 026151008X320507.
Powell,
D. & Dixon, M. (2011). Does SMS text messaging help or harm adults’
knowledge of standard spelling? Journal
of Computer Assisted Learning 27, 58-66. Doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2729.2010.00403.x
Reardon M. (2008) Text messaging explodes in America. CNET Tech News.
23 September. Available at: http:// www.cbsnews.com/stories/2008/09/23/tech/cnettechnews/
main4471183.shtml
Rogers,
D. (2008). We know what u mean, m8. Innit? The
Times Educational Supplement. 12 December. Retrieved from: http://www.tes.co.uk/article.aspx?storycode=6006244
Thurlow,
C. (2003). Generation txt? The sociolinguistics of young people’s text
messaging. Discourse Analysis Online.
Retrieved from: http://extra.shu.ac.uk/daol/articles/v1/n1/a3/thurlow2002003-paper.html
Varnhagen
C.K., McFall G.P., Pugh N., Routledge L., Surnida-MacDonald H. & Kwong T.E.
(2010). Lol: new language and spelling in instant messaging. Reading and Writing: An Interdisiplinary
Journal 23, 719-733. Doi: 10.1007/s11145-009-9181-y.
Text messaging is an effective way for small business owners to communicate with their clients. This is called text messaging marketing, the trend for many businesses today.
ReplyDeleteGainesville mobile marketing strategy